Did human intelligence peak thousands of years ago?

pygmy hunter along a stream
According to at least one Stanford geneticist, humans have been on a cerebral backslide with the rise of modern agriculture and the loss of many hunter-gatherer societies. See our image gallery ofgenetics at work.
© Martin Harvey/CORBIS

For years, researchers, politicians and cranky uncles have held forth on the source of society's looming downfall. Comic books and pulps rot our brains, television is a vast wasteland and the Internet scatters our senses [source:Carr]. Now, just when matters couldn't sound any direr, along comes geneticist Gerald Crabtree of Stanford University to tell us that civilization itself is killing our cognition and extinguishing our emotional stability.

It's enough to make you want to watch "Idiocracy" and drink Brawndo all day.

Advertisement

But perhaps we're getting ahead of ourselves. After all, attempts to unravel the genetics woven into intelligence predate Gregor Mendel's work on plant hybrids, yet we remain in the early stages of the science. Meanwhile, related fields continue their century-long struggle to disentangle intelligence's many definitions, measurements and influences.

Perhaps that's why Crabtree's idea sounds, to many scientists' ears, a bit impudent.

The Stanford geneticist thinkshuman intelligenceis a fragile business, less a robust network and more a genetic Rube Goldberg machine. Unless its components -- a Crabtree-estimated group of 2,000-5,000 key genes -- are intensely selected for in a population, the generation-by-generation buildup of detrimental mutations will doom us to dimness. By his calculations, we've been on a cerebral slide ever since we discovered agriculture and began to form sedentary societies [sources:Crabtree;Crabtree].

According to Crabtree, a hunter-gatherer paid the ultimate penalty for folly by either starving or becoming lion chow. Today's blundering businessperson seems to bear out thePeter Principleby failing upward, getting rich, acquiring a mate and passing on rather baseline brainpower to their children [sources:Crabtree;Crabtree].

To some colleagues, however, this hypothesis has more holes than a ... really, really holey thing. They point out that, on top of the hitches facing any sweeping genetic-intelligence hypothesis -- unclear genetic links, uncertain environmental roles and so on -- Crabtree's big idea has a significant scientific snag: It not only lacks proof, it also lacks any clear means of being proven [sources:Britannica;Chabris et al.;Connor]. As such, it's less a hypothesis and more an interesting notion.

关于社会所谓的负面影响,或者说是ites neural geneticist Kevin J. Mitchell of the Smurfit Institute of Genetics, "No evidence is offered for this idea, which contradicts models suggesting just the opposite: that the complexities of social interactions in human societies were a main driver of increasing intelligence" [source:Mitchell].

Advertisement

Mutations, Degradations and Scholarly Persuasions

Although he says that his ideas arise from anthropology and neurobiology, Crabtree's argument mostly comes down to genetics.

Here's the gist: We all know that genes are little DNA snippets that perform many vital functions in building this biological contraption known as "us," including passing down traits from our parents. Each gene type comes in several flavors calledalleles. For example, a gene (one of many) related to height might code for either tallness or shortness. We inherit half of our genes from each parent, and some dominant alleles, such as brown eyes, tend to, well, dominate over recessive ones, like blue eyes.

Advertisement

Sometimes an allele undergoes an abrupt, random change called amutation.Somatic mutationsaffect a single organism -- you, for example -- whilegerm-line mutationscan be passed on to offspring. Although most mutations are harmful, in rare cases, one can increase a creature's fitness to live on and breed. Hence, mutation is the engine that drives evolution and natural selection.

基于研究的知识缺陷的链接ed to genes on the X chromosome (orXLIDssuch as fragile X syndrome), Crabtree estimates that at least 10 percent of human genes participate in intelligence and believes that every single one of them plays a vital role in intellectual and emotional function. Intelligence, for Crabtree, is a genetic house of cards, and a bad mutation in even one gene can bring the whole affair tumbling down [sources:Crabtree;Crabtree].

To make matters worse, intelligence is particularly susceptible to harmful mutations and is not readily passed down to children, says the geneticist, which means you need a strong selective force in play if you want to keep your bean keen from generation to generation. Between 50,000 and 500,000 years ago, he argues, the unrelenting struggle for survival put hunter-gatherers under pressure aplenty, but more stable agricultural societies selected for traits that better met the needs of their way of life, such as resistance to the diseases it gave rise to. As for modern society, its various safety nets, though desirable, enable people across the intelligence spectrum to breed. Crabtree offers no evidence to back up these assertions [sources:Crabtree;Crabtree;Mitchell].

In short, intellect and emotional maturity are as fragile as a dream, and we are in for a rude awakening.

Crabtree's hypothesis appears to embrace all flavors of intelligence and emotional development. For example, he includes the "intuitive ... non-verbal comprehension of things such as the aerodynamics and gyroscopic stabilization of a spear while hunting" [source:Crabtree]. This strategy makes his claim hard to prove or refute. After all, other factors involved in survival, such as coordination, training or social qualities, might also map to intelligence -- or fail to, depending on which definition better supports Crabtree's thesis.

This vagueness is part of the problem. Scientists, of course, need to carefully define their terms, specify their problems and outline the criteria by which further research might support or refute their hypotheses. On these grounds, and others, Crabtree's critics take him to task.

Advertisement

The Critics Have a Brain to Pick

Critics fault Crabtree for his loose phrasing, sparse proof and reliance on "it stands to reason" arguments. They also disagree with his readings of certain genetic, neurobiological and anthropological studies [sources:Kalinka et al.;Mitchell]. In short, the head bone of contention extends beyond Crabtree's claim of brittle braininess to the very essence of his evidence.

例如,瑰柏翠驳斥了语言的作用expanding the frontal cortex and endocranial volume -- two changes vital to the evolution of abstract thought that he says occurred 50,000-500,000 years ago. But Alex Kalinka and his colleagues at the Max Planck Institute argue that language capacity stretches back much further, to when modern humans split off fromNeanderthals, which could suggest an early, language-related ballooning of the brain [source:Kalinka et al.].

Advertisement

Mitchell, meanwhile, defends society's role inbolstering brainpower. What Crabtree views as a crèche that coddles the less clever, Mitchell sees as the very crucible from which acumen arises, pressured by the complexities intrinsic in social dealings [source:Mitchell]. Crabtree replies that societal impacts -- including environmental, nutritional, perinatal and educational improvements -- merely offset genetic decline [source:Crabtree].

On that topic, Kalinka and company dismiss Crabtree's XLID-based estimate of 2,000-5,000 intelligence deficiency genes. Here's why: The X chromosome boasts more than its share of brain-expressed genes and virtually bristles with links to neurological genetic disorders. In other words, any estimate based on the X chromosome should skew quite high and therefore offer a poor basis for estimation [source:Kalinka et al.].

Returning to the easily broken heart of the matter, Mitchell attacks Crabtree's idea of delicate intelligence on the ground that it would work only if mutations affected humans not as individuals, but as a species, which isn't the case. If it were, he argues, every trait touching on our fitness would degrade over time as we accumulated one nasty mutation after another (sidebar). Not only does this not occur, says Mitchell, but intellectual deficiency genes (ID) tie into a vast web of other genes whose mutations do not cause ID -- a far more robust system than Crabtree describes [source:Mitchell].

Finally, say critics, competing models of genetic development strongly argue for intellectual resilience. Intelligence might not derive from genes that specifically code for it, they say, but rather arise as a side effect of general fitness, a hitchhiker on the coattails of other selected-for traits (or vice versa). Such a model would help explain why intelligence often goes hand-in-hand with various physical and mental health characteristics, including cardiovascular and psychiatric diseases [sources:Deary;Kalinka et al.;Mitchell;Yeo et al.].

In the end, Crabtree himself views his fragile intellect idea as a useful hypothesis, one he admits is in need of greater study and substantiation [source:Crabtree]. Meanwhile, the debate itself underlines just how complex our genetic dance might be -- and how much work remains to be done.

Advertisement

Lots More Information

Author's Note: Did human intelligence peak thousands of years ago?

One troubling aspect of any argument for genetic degradation is the line of thinking it sometimes inspires. Indeed, the very fact that Crabtree repeatedly says that science and knowledge will lead to a solution "by social and morally acceptable means" suggest that he, too, was haunted by the specter of eugenics or, at least, genetic engineering.

Related Articles

  • A.D.A.M. "Sex-linked Recessive -- Overview." University of Maryland Medical Center. http://www.umm.edu/ency/article/002051.htm
  • Carr, Nicholas. "How the Internet is Making Us Stupid." The Telegraph (UK). Aug, 27, 2010. http://www.telegraph.co.uk/technology/internet/7967894/How-the-Internet-is-making-us-stupid.html
  • Connor, Steve. "Human Intelligence 'Peaked Thousands of Years Ago and We've Been on an Intellectual and Emotional Decline Ever Since.'" http://www.independent.co.uk/news/science/human-intelligence-peaked-thousands-of-years-ago-and-weve-been-on-an-intellectual-and-emotional-decline-ever-since-8307101.html
  • Crabtree, Gerald R. "Our Fragile Intellect: Part I." Trends in Genetics. Vol. 29, no. 1. Page 1. January 2013. http://download.cell.com/trends/genetics/pdf/PIIS0168952512001588.pdf
  • Crabtree, Gerald R. "Our Fragile Intellect: Part II." Trends in Genetics. Vol. 29, no. 1. Page 3. January 2013. http://download.cell.com/trends/genetics/pdf/PIIS016895251200159X.pdf
  • Crabtree, Gerald R. " Our Fragile Intellect: Response to Dr. Mitchell." Trends in Genetics. Vol. 29, no. 2. Page 60. February 2013. http://download.cell.com/trends/genetics/pdf/PIIS0168952512002090.pdf
  • Crabtree, Gerald R. " Rethinking Our Intellectual Origins: Response to Kalinka et al." Trends in Genetics. Vol. 29, no. 3. Page 127. March 2013. http://download.cell.com/trends/genetics/pdf/PIIS0168952513000255.pdf
  • Deary, Ian J. "Intelligence." Annual Review of Psychology. Vol. 63. Page 453.
  • Encyclopedia Britannica. "Human Intelligence." http://www.britannica.com/EBchecked/topic/289766/human-intelligence
  • Everding, Gerry. "Brain Imaging Can Predict How Intelligent You Are, Study Finds." July 31, 2012. http://news.wustl.edu/news/Pages/24068.aspx
  • Galton F. "Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry into its Laws and Consequences." London, McMillan/Fontana. 1869.
  • Gardner, Howard. "Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences." Basic Books. 2004.
  • Gardner, Howard. "Are there Additional Intelligences? The Case for Naturalist, Spiritual, and Existential Intelligences." In J. Kane (Ed.), Education, Information, and Transformation. Merrill-Prentice Hall. 1998.
  • Gilman, Lynn and Jonathan Plucker. "The Theory of Multiple Intelligences." Jan. 14, 2013. http://www.intelltheory.com/mitheory.shtml
  • Kalinka, Alex, Iva Kelava and Eric Lewitus. "Our Robust Intellect." Trends in Genetics. Vol. 29, no. 3. Page 125. March 2013. http://download.cell.com/trends/genetics/pdf/PIIS0168952513000206.pdf
  • Mitchell, Kevin J. "Genetic Entropy and the Human Intellect." Trends in Genetics. Vol. 29, no. 2. Page 59. February 2013. http://download.cell.com/trends/genetics/pdf/PIIS0168952512001941.pdf
  • Naik, Guatam. "A Genetic Code for Genius?" The Wall Street Journal. Feb. 15, 2013. http://online.wsj.com/article/SB10001424127887324162304578303992108696034.html
  • Singer, Emily. "Brain Images Reveal the Secret to Higher IQ." Technology Review. March 24, 2009. http://www.technologyreview.com/news/412678/brain-images-reveal-the-secret-to-higher-iq/
  • Tayyari, Farnoosh. "The Genetic Basis of Intelligence." Science Creative Quarterly. August 2004. http://www.scq.ubc.ca/the-genetic-basis-of-intelligence/
  • Thurstone, L. L. "Primary Mental Abilities." University of Chicago Press. 1938.
  • University of Manchester. "Research Reveals Genetic Link to Human Intelligence." ScienceDaily. Aug. 12, 2011. http://www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2011/08/110811215420.htm
  • 唷,罗纳德·A。,史蒂文·w·Gangestad和罗伯特J。Thoma. "Developmental Instability and Individual Variation in Brain Development: Implications for the Origin of Neurodevelopmental Disorders." Current Directions in Psychological Science. Vol. 16, no. 5. Page 245. October 2007.

Advertisement

Loading...